• Awareness
• Conscious
• Unconscious
• Self/Others
• Comparative information
• Frequency of comparison
When first asked, 75% of the children interviewed said they compared their schoolwork with others in their class. A similar response (71%) was evident via questionnaire. The following response was typical:
“yeh … I like to see whether people have the same ideas as me … I like comparing my work but I don’t always do it” (boy, medium ability-group).
A difference according to ability groups was observed with 100% of the children assigned to the low-ability groups acknowledging their use of social comparison compared to 58% of those in middle group and 67% in the high group. Not only does this signal a difference in the quantity of social comparisons according to ability grouping but also a difference in the implicit/explicit quality of social comparisons according to ability appeared to be occurring. Although all pupils were able to talk of themselves in relation to others the low ability pupils used comparison more frequently, to a greater extent to judge their own performance and appeared to use social comparison in a more deliberate and explicit way.
Analysis of the interview data of those pupils who failed to immediately acknowledge their use of social comparison appeared to reveal an unconscious/automatic nature of social comparison for children in other ability groups. For example, despite denying that they used social comparison when asked directly, all pupils’ responses to indirect questioning suggested that they did in fact compare. For example, the quotes below shows the different responses to interview questions from the same pupil. When asked “Do you compare your schoolwork with others?” the pupil’s response was negative. Yet when replying to the question “How do you judge how good or bad you are at schoolwork”? the same child’s answer showed that they compared often.
Do you compare your schoolwork with others?
Carol: Not normally no
How do you judge how good or bad you are at schoolwork?
Carol: ….. well …. I know I have done my best and I can’t do any more so I don’t judge my ability on what others have done but I do like to see what others have done especially in science and I compare with them quite often.
What is striking about the pupil’s words quoted above is how the curriculum area in which she used social comparison was identified. Thirty-two of the 36 children interviewed spontaneously identified the specific subject or subjects in which they more likely to compare their work. As in the quantitative questionnaire, Maths and Science were nominated most frequently by pupils. Pupils of different ability groups appeared to be similar in their responses in that not only did the children identify a specific curriculum area but many of the pupils gave a clear reason as to why they were more likely to compare in this subject than in others. The reasons given fell into two categories; enjoyment (I like/don’t like the topic) and perceived ability (I am good/not good at it), as illustrated by the quotes below. These findings relate to the Local Dominance Effect ( Buckingham and Alicke, 2002 ; Zell and Alicke, 2010 ) (see section “Discussion”).
Sue: I compare sometimes in particular lessons more in English as I am not so good at English and other people are better than me.
Paul: Yes, in Art because most people are better than me.
James: I like to compare in lessons where maybe I could have done better, my weaker subject.
Sally: more in science. It is my strongest subject; I really like it and I want to do well.
Tom: I compare in all the subjects I don’t understand. I get confused in English and it’s boring and the teacher is rubbish so I don’t like that subject and I compare in that.
Children were asked to nominate one person that they normally compare their schoolwork with (SCT). This comparison is referred to here as an everyday ‘real’ comparison as children are not forced in selecting a preferred comparison given a hypothetical scenario but instead to select a person in their class that they actually do compare with; a real comparison. The nomination of one self-selected SCT allowed further examination of the qualities of self-selected comparisons.
Friendships were a key factor in the choice of SCT in both the quantitative questionnaire (see Table 1 ) and in the qualitative interviews, regardless of ability group. That said, the interviews revealed that other requirements of the comparison target were sought after in addition to friendship. Pupils wanted their SCT to provide help or be good at a particular subject.
Julia: she is a friend and she sometimes gets it right better than others.
Harriet: she is a close friend.....she struggles at maths and English but she is better at science.....well I think so. We do work well together.
Sam: he is a friend and he listens to me and says what I got wrong, others like Ben just mess around.
Whilst pupils were easily able to identify the SCT they compare with the most, in the interview’s children’s ‘free responses’ provided evidence of comparison with more than one SCT and occasionally different SCT’s for different topics. No child referred to more than three SCT’s. Seating/table assignment was a key influence on self-selection of the SCT as the quotes below illustrate:
Sarah: I compare to the person I sit next to who is a friend. I compare more if I sit next to a friend. Only if I get stuck will I compare with someone who is not a friend.
Jack: in science we can sit next to whoever we like … If I sit next to a friend I am more likely to compare although if I don’t understand then I’d compare with someone in the group even if they are not a friend.
While ‘free choice’ self-selection of SCT was preferred by all children, it appeared that to some extent the SCT is a forced option rather than a deliberate choice in the groups. Many pupils spoke of being allocated places by their teachers and as a result having to compare with the person sitting next to them. The forced comparisons were associated with lower frequency of comparisons as illustrated in the quotes above, with pupils saying they only compare if they are struggling and more likely to compare if they can choose a friend to compare with.
As previously discussed, the direction of comparison can be upwards, downwards, or horizontal. That is, pupils can compare themselves with someone above their ability level, below their ability level or with someone at the same level of ability. Different methods were used to gauge direction of comparison. The reaction approach used comparative rating to ask pupils to rate themselves against others and thus explored pupil’s perception of relative ability. It is important to bear in mind that this is not actual ability, but perceived ability as judged by each pupil. This method suggested that pupils compared themselves upwards (69%) or horizontally (25%) (25 pupils said they were the same as the rest of the class, 9 said better and 2 said worse). The quantitative questionnaire confirmed this upwards/horizontal finding. The mean score of 3.61 indicated that on the whole pupils said their comparison target was between “the same as the rest of the class” (point 3) and “better than the rest of the class” (point 4).
The selection approach confirmed the upward direction of comparison using the adapted rank order paradigm pupils were given a hypothetical scenario and asked to select a SCT based on rank (see quantitative measures for detailed description). When presented with this scenario 31 of the 36 children (86%) chose to view a score above their own (above the middle) and 5 chose to view a score below their own (3 of which were from the low ability group). Similar results were found quantitatively with 84% choosing to view above the middle rank (see Table 3 ). Due to the scenario forcing children to choose a different rank to the one they occupied there was no specific measurement of horizontal comparisons in this approach, however, aggregating scores around the middle revealed that 44.6% choose to compare close to the middle (indicating horizontal comparisons) 49.6% of children choose top bands indicating a strong upwards preference. Thus closer inspection suggests preference for upwards or horizontal social comparison.
The narration approach confirmed the upwards/horizontal direction with approx. half of the children comparing upwards and half horizontal. When asked about the ability level of their SCT, 47% ( n = 17) of those interviewed said that their SCT was better than most of the class and 53% ( n = 19) said that the SCT was the same as the rest of the class. Ability group did make a difference here. The SCT for low and high ability groups tended to be the same as the rest of the class whereas the medium ability groups tended to compare themselves with someone that they perceived to be better than the rest of the class. In other words, the direction of comparison was horizontal for the high and low abilities, and upwards for the medium ability children.
As discussed above, all children judged their SCT to be better than or the same as the rest of the class. These performance judgments not only provided an indication of the direction of comparison but also allowed one to explore how pupils judged performance. Three reasons were given by the pupils as to how they were able to gauge the ability of the SCT namely: knowledge of scores, observation of pieces of work, and viewing class discussions participation such as public questioning.
In terms of their own ability in relation to the rest of the class, the high ability group tended to judge themselves in the top two quarters of the class, the medium group judged themselves to be in the middle two quarters and the low ability group judged themselves to be across the 2nd, 3rd, and 4th quarters. When asked how pupils gauge their own performance the response was varied and less certain than when asked about the SCT. Half the pupils couldn’t seem to answer the question and gave vague responses. This is perhaps not unexpected. Pupils gauged others’ academic ability on what they saw and heard in the classroom and as such they understood that this was how they judged others’ ability. In contrast pupils could not state how they came to know things about themselves. By the age of 11 children would have had a wealth of experiences on which to base their self-perceptions and perhaps found it difficult to select one or two reasons. This occurred throughout the interview. Pupils were happy to provide a judgment regarding themselves but were either slow or unable to express how they came to that judgment.
Paul: I don’t struggle but I am not really, really good.
Sue: I don’t know. I know I am ok and so are the other people in my class.
To some extent the above implies that children are overtly engaging in social comparison and thus can describe the reasons for such a judgment but are unaware of the impact it plays upon themselves. The inference of deliberate comparison with others was confirmed when the discussion moved away from general judgments of ability to more specific judgments of pieces of work. Pupils were asked to think how they might judge their work in class if they were given a task to complete today. All pupils offered a more detailed account with pupils either describing how the teacher would say or do things that would inform them of their performance, how they would use the content of their own work to judge their performance or how they would compare their work with others to judge the quality of their work. Children in the low ability groups were more likely to refer to comparison with peers (83%) than children in the medium (42%) or high ability (33%) groups.
All pupils identified the emotional impact of social comparison. Pupils’ responses fell into three categories: dual emotions, confidence or negative emotions. Dual emotions refer to the differing emotions, both positive and negative, because of social comparison. Approximately 40% of those interviewed discussed the contented, pleased reaction gained from superior performance compared to a comparison target (SCT) and the disapproving, anxious feelings exhibited as a result of inferior performance. Moreover, as the quote below highlights children are using the SCT as a way to judge the standard of their schoolwork as social comparison theory ( Festinger, 1954 ) posits:
William: Ok … if mine is just as good as his then I feel pleased because I know I have reached a good standard. If his is better than mine I feel jealous …I think that I need to try better next time.
Scott: When I get a better grade, I feel sort of proud of myself, not like boastful or anything but happy.
The quantitative results highlight that comparing favorably evokes positive emotional responses in most children (see Table 4 ). 84% of children surveyed said they would feel positive if their work was better than their peers. For 14 of those interviewed, social comparison with their classmates produced only feelings of increased confidence. These pupils tended to mention the confidence and peace of mind gained through social comparisons in that they reduced the uncertainty involved in completing a piece of work at school.
Mark: More confident I suppose that he has the same ideas.
A small number of pupils (17%: 6 of the 36 interviewed) only saw the negative aspects of social comparison and discussed negative emotions exclusively. In contrast in the quantitative questionnaire only 1% of children reported negative responses to both upward and downward comparison ( Table 4 ). The qualitative interviews did aim to unpack how the children experienced social comparisons and thus this difference can be explained methodologically. All of these pupils who spoke only of negative emotions from social comparisons were from the low ability group. Those children assigned to low ability groups experienced more feelings of negativity as a result of their social comparisons than those in medium or high groups.
Amy: It makes me feel stupid, jealous and frustrated.
Max: I just hate myself for being so stupid.
As the quote above illustrates, for some children within the low ability groups social comparison with others can be a terrible experience.
The findings presented here provide an indication of the vital role social comparison plays in the primary school classroom. Social comparison was found to be a highly prevalent practice experienced by all pupils regardless of ability group. These findings support previous research which shows that the classroom environment is the perfect place for comparisons ( Dijkstra et al., 2008 ) given the extensive source of social comparative information ( Levine, 1983 ; Buunk et al., 2005 ) and the evaluative atmosphere (e.g., Pepitone, 1972 ; Levine, 1983 ). While all children did compare, a difference according to ability group was evident with children assigned to the low-ability groups using social comparison more frequently, consciously and deliberately. The reason for such a finding could lie in the underlying motivation for comparison. Researchers such as Kruglanksi (1989) and Wheeler et al. (1997) have posited that uncertainty motivates comparison. Indeed, most of the pupils in the current study identified level of perceived ability as being one of the key motivators toward comparison. Individuals seek social comparison in situations when other routes to self-assessment are not feasible to reduce uncertainty and gain valid appraisals of themselves. It is possible, therefore, that children working in low ability groups actively seek comparative information in order to reduce uncertainty whereas pupils in high ability groups are either more certain of themselves or are able to use other forms of self-assessment. Indeed this finding resonates with research which has found that subordinate and dominate group status influenced the impact of social comparison on self-esteem and that dismissal of comparison information as a mechanism of self-protection was reserved for those of dominant groups ( Martinot and Redersdorff, 2003 , 2006 ). Thus, high ability group pupils as members of the dominant group, unlike the low ability subordinate group, appeared able to dismiss social comparative information.
Exploring children’s acknowledgment/awareness of social comparison, the present study found that a large majority of children acknowledged their use of social comparison, with similar results exhibited on both quantitative and qualitative measures. Whilst it was not surprising that all children compared, the acknowledgment of such engagement was unexpected given previous research which had indicated that pupils were reluctant to admit to comparing themselves with others ( Ruble, 1983 ). However, this may be explained methodologically. Previous research on social comparison has tended to involve quantitative measures only and/or laboratory settings ( Wood, 2000 ). It is, therefore, possible that the informal, non-judgmental nature of the individual qualitative interviews conducted within the school setting in the present study may have contributed to this finding. Indeed, particular thought was given to the framing of the question regarding social comparison since pilot testing the meaning of comparison had revealed that pupils of 10 and 11 years of age did not differentiate between “comparison” and “copying.” Copying is frowned upon in school and thus admitting to such an act would tend to have negative consequences. To avoid this, questionnaires and interviews included an everyday example of social comparison and a reminder that it did not refer to copying. This meant that pupils were able to discuss the act of comparing their work with others with a clear understanding of the meaning of comparison. This may have been an important first step for the children, enabling them to understand and discuss when, where and with whom they compared their work and the effects of such comparisons.
It should be noted that pupils who didn’t immediately acknowledge their engagement in social comparison did so after a ‘warm up’ to the interview. For example, the same child would initially report not comparing with peers and then later in the interview describe their frequent use of social comparison. Reconciling this incongruence, it may be a social desirability response as discussed earlier or it could potentially signal an unconscious element of social comparisons. As Wolff et al. (2020) note unconscious social comparisons have been reported in previous research (e.g., Mussweiler et al., 2004 ; Alicke and Zell, 2008 ) yet research is extremely limited to such an extent it is not mentioned in Gerber et al. (2018) recent review, thus further research on unconscious social comparisons is warranted. Alternatively, the ability for the children to ‘warm up’ to the interview could be the reason for the incongruence in response. As discussed above, a key strength of the qualitative aspect of this study was that it enabled exploration of social comparison within different ability groups in a safe environment for children to express themselves, without danger of influence of other children. This ‘safe space’ could have allowed children to overcome any initial reluctance to and feel comfortable to discuss their engagement in social comparison. This would be very reasonable given the negative emotional impact social comparisons can have on a child. Research conducted in naturalistic settings emphasizes understanding the context of the study and tailoring the design ( Argio et al., 2019 ). The qualitative aspect of this study appears to have fulfilled this by allowing discussion and freedom for children to fully understand and engage in the topic, enabling us to gain greater knowledge about the nature of real comparisons in the classroom.
Findings in relation to who children compared with (the SCT) concur with previous research that has emphasized the salience of SCT’s ( Blanton et al., 1999 ; Huguet et al., 2001 ; Liem et al., 2013 ). The findings here show that the SCT was chosen deliberately by pupils according to their qualities as a friend and characteristics which would render them helpful, supportive, or academically useful. Friendships thus are very important yet often overlooked or discounted when teachers form ability groups. Indeed, in the classroom environment children couldn’t always choose who they compared with and grouping, table assignment and seating impacted comparison frequency and comparison outcome. If children couldn’t compare with their self-selected/chosen SCT then the frequency and motive for comparison altered and consequently the impact of the comparison changed. This is important as it shows the influence teachers can have on social comparisons through grouping practices. Children’s ‘free’ responses (in response to open questions about social comparison) tended to focus on local comparisons with one or two friends. They didn’t actively seek comparisons with large number of peers or groups of children, and at no time did any pupil mention comparisons with the class. Children placed great emphasize on actively seeking social comparison with a select small number of other children and these social comparisons were very important to the children. These findings support the Local Dominance Effect ( Buckingham and Alicke, 2002 ; Zell and Alicke, 2010 ) which shows that comparisons with few individuals has a greater influence on self-assessments than comparisons with larger aggregates. Indeed, the current study found that whilst pupils did talk about what they compare and why they compare, who they compare with was discussed by pupils far more and thus appeared to exert a greater influence on the child.
Examining what pupils are comparing, the present study found that children readily identified the curriculum subject they were more likely to compare and give reasons for their motivation to compare. These findings indicate greater reliance on dimensional comparison than social comparison. According to dimensional comparison research ( Möller and Marsh, 2013 ; Strickhouser and Zell, 2015 ; Wolff et al., 2018 ) individuals compare their achievements or abilities in one subject (e.g., science) to their achievements or abilities in other subject (e.g., maths). Dimensional comparisons are made between domains/subjects and social comparisons within the same domain. The findings here concur with that in found other studies of dimensional comparison with children feeling better about (liking) a subject in which their performance is superior. Moreover, a recent research on academic enjoyment ( Boliver and Capsada-Munsech, 2021 ), determined that liking school subjects in ages 7–11 years was vital for academic achievement.
Overall, the findings here showed a preference to compare upwards, resonating with previous research in classrooms ( Blanton et al., 1999 ; Huguet et al., 2001 ; Gerber et al., 2018 ). However, there was a strong (and near equal) preference for horizontal social comparison. The direction of comparison is important for children’s self-evaluation. As discussed in the introduction, according to social comparison theory ( Festinger, 1954 ) people use other people as the standard to judge themselves against. Achieving a grade C could be viewed as being very good if one’s peers achieved grade E, but poor if one’s classmates achieved grade A. If a child compares ‘upwards’ with someone above themselves (e.g., the grade A classmate), they can either think they are not as good as the other child or identify with the other child and think they can improve. The findings here showed a convergent of results across different measures, when looking at the aggregates, confirming a tendency to compare upwards. However, the preference for horizontal comparisons was also strong and the interviews showed a slightly higher preference for horizontal over upwards comparison (horizontal 53%/upwards 47%). Unpacking this further we see a difference by ability group, with the high and low abilities engaging in horizontal comparisons, and the medium ability upwards comparisons. Of course, it is possible that this finding is due to reporting bias. High ability pupils may not have felt comfortable labeling their comparison target “worse” and low ability pupils may not have wanted to acknowledge their inferiority by admitting that their SCT was “better.” That said, given the ‘safe space’ of the interviews discussed above this seems unlikely.
Drawing on previous research on groups ( Martinot and Redersdorff, 2006 ; Alicke et al., 2010 ; Martinot et al., 2020 ), Social Identity Theory ( Tajfel and Turner, 1986 ) and self-evaluation maintenance theory (SEM) a more likely explanation explored here is that group membership is influencing children’s self-perceptions and psychological protection is at play impacting social comparisons. According to Social Identity Theory ( Tajfel and Turner, 1986 ) individuals can think of themselves in terms of an ‘individual’ self or their ‘group’ self. This can vary according to context and thus as individual can exhibit different aspects of the self in different settings. In some circumstances people may prefer to compare themselves with members of the same group (intragroup) especially if they compare favorably to the group as a whole to such an extent that they ignore any out-group standards and base their self-evaluation on their position within the group ( Major, 1994 ). Indeed, Alicke et al. (2010) found that simply dividing students into arbitrary groups produced a tendency for students to focus on local information to categorize their standing in a group which was used for self-evaluations. In the present study, the high ability pupils as members of the dominant group, are motivated to retain their position, they can dismiss comparison information and as there isn’t a higher group, they compare horizontally with other group members. The medium ability aspires to be in the dominant group, assimilating to be like those above them and thus compare upwards. The low ability children, as members of the subordinate group cannot compare downwards as they occupy the lowest rank. The question is why aren’t these children comparing upwards? Self-evaluation maintenance theory ( Tesser, 1988 ) assumes that all individuals try to maintain a positive self-evaluation and utilize alternative ways in which social comparative information is received and processed. While upwards comparisons can promote growth and learning and thus have positive impact, if an individual focuses on the contrasts (differences) between them and the target it can lead to feelings of inferiority, jealousy and inadequacy (see earlier discussion). Given the impact on emotional impact of social comparison found in this study, it is posited that the low ability pupils are actively seeking comparative information horizontally in order to protect themselves against the negative impact of ‘unattainable’ upwards comparisons. This resonates with research on psychological disengagement mechanisms with adolescents where students from disadvantaged groups discount or and devalue grades or unhappiness due to a unfavorable social comparisons to protect themselves from negative outcomes ( Martinot et al., 2020 ).
There is no disputing the emotional/psychological impact of social comparisons. All pupils were to some extent affected by comparisons with their peers, although this was more extreme and more frequent for those assigned to low ability. The mixture of emotions experienced by pupils through social comparison showed the turmoil and insecurity that some pupils faced on a daily basis. Having a child within the interviews admit to hating themselves because of comparisons with their classmates is an illustration of the profound negative impact it can have on a child’s life yet it appeared that this was accepted as part and parcel of the social interaction that occurred within ability groups.
Despite the strength of the mixed methods in the present study, there were limitations. Only 12 primary schools in similar geographical location were included in this study which narrows the cultural diversity and generalizability of the study. The qualitative, ecologically valid, naturalistic aspect of this study is its strength, designed to explore the nature of social comparisons in the classroom. The quantitative data enhanced the descriptions, enabled comparisons, and illustrated and confirmed the qualitative findings yet could have added more depth had the quantitative component been given more equal weight in the design. Moreover, the anonymous/de-identified design of the quantitative questionnaires (ethical requirements) did not permit direct matching to individual qualitative data which would have provided additional triangulation to support findings. Adapting the interview design so that the questionnaire is completed during one of the interviews would overcome such a constraint. Alternatively, a sequential mixed methods design may have merit. Having the quantitative first and then using interviews to explain the data in a sequential explanatory design would allow selection of children on the basis of their responses.
Further limitations of the quantitative measures involve the use of rank order scenario. While it captured, upwards/downwards direction of social comparison and near/extreme ranks, the forced method required children to choose a different rank to the one they occupied which didn’t permit measurement of horizontal/lateral social comparison. Given that horizontal comparisons were salient in the qualitative data, being able to capture this quantitatively would be beneficial. As discussed earlier, the measurement of social comparison is complex and one of the key criticisms of forced comparisons is that they don’t relate to the ‘real world’ ( Wood, 2000 ), with researchers cautioning against their generalization ( Dijkstra et al., 2008 ; Gerber et al., 2018 ; Boissicat et al., 2020 ). Thus, the limitation of quantitative forced comparisons highlighted here concurs with previous research and leads us to advocate for mixed methods and qualitative research to build on the existing quantitative literature and forward the field.
The findings presented here provide evidence of the vital role social comparison plays in the primary school classroom. Social comparison amongst 10–11-year-old children was found to be a highly prevalent daily aspect of classroom life experienced by all pupils regardless of ability group. Children engaged in social comparisons at different levels, yet the importance of friendships and local comparisons (where children actively sought comparisons with a very small number of friends) were far more important to children (the Local Dominance Effect). The fluency with which children discussed comparisons with peers was staggering and tends to suggest a strong prevalence of this of method for self-evaluation. This was particularly salient for children assigned to low-ability groups who were more vulnerable to the negative effects of social comparison.
While all children compared, children assigned to low-ability groups used social comparison more frequently, consciously and deliberately, actively seeking horizontal intra-group comparisons. In doing so they avoided upwards comparisons thereby protecting themselves from unfavorable comparisons with others. One explanation explored within this paper is that group membership/group standing influences children’s self-perceptions and self-evaluations to such an extent that psychological protection mechanisms kick in and alter/impact social comparisons processes. Yet it is forwarded here that while these strategies may provide psychological ‘protection’, they may also be the roadblock in the path to better outcomes for such children. Ability groupings mean that pupils are forced to compare with same level pupils. For low ability children, whilst this may ‘protect’ children from negative comparisons and self-evaluations, it means they miss out on comparisons with more able pupils. If we accept that social comparisons can facilitate learning, encourage growth and self-improvement, then these children are missing out on the very practices that could support them. Seeing other children solve problems, explain their thinking and learning through shared discussion is vital and so limiting these opportunities for children appears to be misguided.
With some investment in understanding how better to group children (or not group children) and how to remove psychological roadblocks, teachers can set up classroom practices where children don’t have to protect themselves. You only have to look at the affective reactions to social comparisons in this study to see the vital role they play in children’s well-being and emotional stability. All pupils were to some extent affected by comparisons with their peers, negative and positive, although the negative impact was more extreme and frequent for those assigned to low ability groups. Pupil–pupil interaction in the classroom that leads a child to say they hate themselves clearly needs to be addressed.
Teacher education and professional development on this topic could help teachers to identify and support children that rely on self-evaluations of performance based on social comparisons and redirect them to teacher appraisal and feedback. Moreover, given that self-perceptions are related to actual performance, teachers should be alerted to the implications of social comparison for motivation, engagement and achievement. The argument presented here is that teachers need to put a greater emphasis on pupil–pupil interactions and social comparisons when forming within-class ability-groups. The collaborative nature of classrooms today means greater emphasis on talk and discussion for learning and thus greater availability of social comparative information. The groups children are assigned to influences what information children hear or see about their classmates and what teacher input/time they are allocated. Grouping practices including peer–peer interaction, seating, table allocation needs to be considered in terms of social comparison opportunities. Forced comparisons (comparisons chosen by the teacher as opposed free choice self-selected comparisons by the child) altered the frequency and motive for comparisons and thus self-evaluations. Appropriate planning by teachers of these interactions is advocated which take account of the extent to which pupils of different ability groups process and utilize social comparative information and consider grouping children in different ways – so that children work in a range of groups and in particular consider friendships as a way to group children in primary classes. Thus, there is significant value in understanding more about how teachers can harness the power of social comparison to enhance outcomes for children. To achieve this further research is needed to understand the everyday social comparisons children make in the classroom. This study has provided evidence for the importance of capturing qualitative data and creating ‘safe spaces’ and including ‘warmups’ in the interview design. Thus, capturing social comparisons utilizing qualitative and mixed research methods in natural environments is advocated given the depth of understanding it affords.
Ethics statement.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Cambridge. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Part of the book series: Springer International Handbooks of Education ((SIHE))
217 Accesses
Case studies have long been used to research and illuminate the lives of gifted students. One example is the longitudinal study of profoundly gifted students by the first author elaborating the impact of acceleration, non-acceleration, and ability grouping on gifted students academically, socially, and emotionally. In this chapter an overview of ability grouping and acceleration for academically gifted high school students in Australia in relation to their self-esteem will be reiterated. Specifically, a study will be discussed that examined the effects of three interventions: (1) ability grouping, (2) acceleration, and (3) acceleration used concurrently with ability grouping, on the self-esteem of academically gifted high school students. Self-esteem was assessed on three occasions during the school year with three groups of academically gifted Australian students undertaking their first year of secondary school (Year 7): (a) students in comprehensive schools (mixed-ability settings); (b) students in selective high schools (full-time ability grouping in schools serving only gifted students); and (c) students in selective high schools who were also collapsing Years 7 and 8 into a single year—a blend of ability grouping and acceleration. Students in the first two groups experienced a diminution (but not a serious drop) in self-esteem, probably associated with the drop-in status from being the most senior students in a (relatively) small primary school to being one of the youngest students in a school that could be several times larger. By contrast, students in the third group, which was not only experiencing the shift from primary to secondary school status but also telescoping the first two high school years into one, experienced only a modest dip in self-esteem. Their scores started off high and remained high. Some teachers in the schools employing the blend of acceleration and grouping expressed their concern that the use of two treatments rather than one might be ‘a bridge too far’. It does not seem to have been. Rather it seems to be an effective mode of blending two interventions whose effectiveness for supporting gifted students’ self-esteem is already acknowledged. Implications for research and practice that links with current research will be elaborated.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Institutional subscriptions
Assouline, S. G., Colangelo, N., VanTassel-Baska, J., & Lupkowski-Shoplik, A. (Eds.). (2015). A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students. John Templeton Foundation: Connie Belin and Jacqueline N . Iowa City, IA: Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development, The University of Iowa.
Google Scholar
Blackburn, A. M., & Smith, S. R. (2018). Capacities, challenges, and curriculum for Australian learners with exceptional potential for English-language learning. In B. Wallace, D. Sisk, & J. Senior (Eds.), SAGE handbook of gifted and talented education (pp. 357–372). Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.
Chan, L. K. S. (1996). Motivational orientations and metacognitive abilities of intellectually gifted students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 40 (4), 184–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698629604000403
Article Google Scholar
Chichekian, T., & Shore, B. M. (2014). Cognitive characteristics of the gifted reconceptualized in the context of inquiry learning and teaching. In N. Colangelo, S. G. Assouline, & M. U. M. Gross (Eds.), A nation deceived: How schools hold back America’s brightest students (pp. 117–129). Iowa City, IA: John Templeton Foundation: Belin-Blank Center for Gifted and Talented Education, The University of Iowa.
Colangelo, N., Assouline, S. G., & Gross, M. U. M. (2004). A nation deceived: How schools hold back America’s brightest students. In Iowa City, IA: John Templeton Foundation: Belin-Blank Center for Gifted and Talented Education . Iowa City, IA: The University of.
Coopersmith, S. (1981). Self-esteem inventories: Manual . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Costa, A. L., & Kallick, B. (2009). Habits of mind across the curriculum: Practical and creative strategies for teachers . Alexandra, VA: ASCD.
Cross, T., & Cross, J. (2017). Maximizing potential: A school-based conception of psychosocial development. High Ability Studies, 28 (1), 43–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2017.1292896
Dai, D. Y., Moon, S. M., & Feldhusen, J. F. (1998). Achievement motivation and gifted students: A social cognitive perspective. Educational Psychologist, 33 (2–3), 45–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1998.9653290
Dare, L., Nowecki, E. A., & Smith, S. R. (2019). On deciding to accelerate: High-ability students identify key considerations. Gifted Child Quarterly, 63 (3), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986219828073
Dare, L., Smith, S. R., & Nowecki, E. (2016). Parents’ experiences with their children’s grade-based acceleration: Struggles, successes, and subsequent needs. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 25 (2), 6–21.
Elliot, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (1), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.5
Feldhusen, J. F. (Ed.). (1985). Toward excellence in gifted education . Denver, CO: Love.
Figg, S. D., Rogers, K. B., McCormick, J., & Low, R. (2012). Differentiating low performance of the gifted learner: Achieving, underachieving, and selective consuming students. Journal of Advanced Academics, 23 (1), 53–71. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/1932202X11430000
Gagné, F. (2013). The DMGT: Changes within, beneath, and beyond. Talent Development and Excellence, 5 (1), 5 – 19. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285946236_The_DMGT_Changes_Within_Beneath_and_Beyond
Gallagher, S., & Smith, S. R. (2013). Acceleration for talent development: Parents’ and teachers’ perspectives on supporting the social and emotional needs of gifted children. International Journal for Talent Development and Creativity (IJTDC), 1 (2), 97–112.
Gallagher, S., Smith, S. R., & Merrotsy, P. (2011). Teachers’ perceptions of the socioemotional development of intellectually gifted primary aged students and their attitudes towards ability grouping and acceleration. Gifted and Talented International, 26 (1–2), 11–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/15332276.2011.11673585
Gross, M. (1995). Relationships between self-esteem and motivational orientation among gifted students in full-time programs . Paper presented at The Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development, University of Iowa.
Gross, M. U. M. (1989). The pursuit of excellence or the search for intimacy? The forced-choice dilemma of gifted youth. Roeper Review, 11 (4), 189–194. https://doi.org/10.1080/02783198909553207
Gross, M. U. M. (1993). Exceptionally gifted children . London, England: Routledge.
Book Google Scholar
Gross, M. U. M. (1997). How ability grouping turns big fish into little fish or does it? Of optical illusions and optimal environments. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 6 (2), 18–30.
Gross, M. U. M. (1999). Inequity in equity: The paradox of gifted education in Australia. Australian Journal of Education, 43 (1), 87–103. https://doi.org/10.1177/000494419904300107
Gross, M. U. M. (2001). Ability grouping, self-esteem and the gifted: A study of optical illusions and optimal environments. Keynote presentation. In N. Colangelo & S. Assouline (Eds.), Talent development 1V: Proceedings of the 4th Biennial Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development (pp. 59–88). Scottsdale, AZ: Gifted Potential Press.
Gross, M. U. M. (2002). How ability grouping turns big fish into little fish—Or does it? Of optical illusions and optimal environments. In W. Vialle & J. Geake (Eds.), The gifted enigma (pp. 131–163). Sydney, NSW: Hawker Brownlow Education.
Gross, M. U. M. (2004a). Benefits of gifted acceleration. Directions in Education, 13 (18), 2.
Gross, M. U. M. (2004b). Exceptionally gifted children (2nd ed.). London, England: RoutledgeFalmer.
Gross, M. U. M. (2004c). Selective high schools: A mainstream alternative. Understanding Our Gifted, 16 (3), 25–27.
Gross, M. U. M. (2006a). Big fish in little ponds? Motivation, self-esteem, and ability grouping. Understanding Our Gifted, 18 (2), 22–23.
Gross, M. U. M. (2006b). Exceptionally gifted children: Long-term outcomes of academic acceleration and non-acceleration. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 29 (4), 404–429. https://doi.org/10.4219/jeg-2006-247
Gross, M. U. M. (2007a). A longitudinal study of radical acceleration with exceptionally and profoundly gifted children. In K. Kay, D. Robson, & J. F. Brennan (Eds.), High IQ kids (pp. 212–243). Minneapolis, MN: Free Spirit Publishing.
Gross, M. U. M. (2007b). Strangers in a strange land: Gifted indigenous children. Understanding Our Gifted, 20 (1), 26–27.
Gross, M. U. M. (2007c). When not “risking” acceleration is just too great a risk. Understanding Our Gifted, 19 (3), 24–25.
Gross, M. U. M. (2008a). Musings: “Hasten slowly”: Thoughtfully planned acceleration. Understanding Our Gifted, 20 (2), 6–8.
Gross, M. U. M. (2008b). Highly gifted children and adolescents. In C. M. Callahan & J. A. Plucker (Eds.), Critical issues and practices in gifted education: What the research says (pp. 241–251). Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
Gross, M. U. M. (2009). How do intellectually gifted children perceive friendship? Understanding Our Gifted, 21 (4), 23–25.
Gross, M. U. M. (2010). Miraca Gross in her own write: A lifetime in gifted education . Sydney, NSW: GERRIC, UNSW.
Gross, M. U. M. (2015). Characteristics of able, gifted, highly gifted, exceptionally gifted, and profoundly gifted learners. In H. E. Viderger & C. R. Harris (Eds.), Applied practice for educators of gifted and able learners (pp. 3–24). Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers.
Chapter Google Scholar
Gross, M. U. M. (2016). What is acceleration and why would we accelerate a child through school? An Australian Mensa initiative . Australian Mensa Inc. Retrieved from https://www.mensa.org.au/giftedchildren/amii
Gross, M. U. M., Urquhart, R., Doyle, J., Juratowitch, M., & Matheson, G. (2011). Releasing the brakes for high ability learners: Administer, teacher and parent attitudes and beliefs that block or assist the implementation of school policies on academic acceleration . Sydney, NSW: Gifted Education Research, Resource and Information Centre (GERRIC), University of New South Wales. Retrieved from https://education.arts.unsw.edu.au/media/EDUCFile/Releasing_the_Brakes_Overview_A4__Nov2011.pdf
Gross, M. U. M., & Van Vliet, H. E. (2005). Why radical acceleration works and when to use it. Understanding Our Gifted, 17 (2), 22–25.
Jung, J. Y., & Gross, M. U. M. (2015). Radical acceleration. In S. G. Assouline, N. Colangelo, J. VanTassel-Baska, & A. Lupkowski-Shoplik (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students (Vol. 2, pp. 199–208). Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin and Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development, University of Iowa.
Jung, J. Y., & Young, M. (2019). The occupational/career decision-making processes of intellectually gifted adolescents from economically disadvantaged backgrounds: A mixed methods perspective. Gifted Child Quarterly, 63 , 36–57. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986218804575
Jung, J. Y., Young, M., & Gross, M. U. M. (2015). Early collage entrance in Australia. Roeper Review, 37 (1), 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/02783193.2014.976323
Kaman, Y., & Kronborg, L. (2012). Perceptions of learning at a select entry accelerated high school for high ability students. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 21 (2), 47–61. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/288096967_Perceptions_of_learning_at_a_select_entry_accelerated_high_school_for_high_ability_students
Kronborg, L., & Cornejo-Araya, C. A. (2018). Gifted educational provisions for gifted and highly able students in Victorian schools, Australia. Universitas Psychologica, 17 (5), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy17–5.gepg
Kronborg, L., & Plunkett, M. (2012). Examining teacher attitudes and perceptions of teacher competencies required in a new selective high school. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 21 , 33–47. Retrieved from https://search.informit.com.au/documentSummary;dn=098906367453401;res=IELAPA
Kulik, J. A. (1992). An analysis of the research on ability grouping: Historical and contemporary perspectives . Storrs, CT: National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented.
Lassig, C. J. (2009). Teachers’ attitudes towards the gifted: The importance of professional development and school culture. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 18 (2), 32–42.
Long, L. C., Barnett, K., & Rogers, K. B. (2015). Exploring the relationship between principal, policy, & gifted program scope and quality. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 38 , 118–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162353215578279
Lupkowsi-Shoplik, A., Assouline, S., & Colangelo, N. (2015). Whole-grade acceleration: Grade skipping and early entrance to kindergarten or first grade. In S. Assouline, N. Colangelo, J. VanTassel-Baska, & A. Lupkowski-Shoplik (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students (pp. 53–72). Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Centre for Gifted Education and Talent Development.
Makel, M., Lee, S., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Putallaz, M. (2012). Changing the pond, not the fish: Following high ability students across different educational environments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104 (3), 778–792. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027558
Masters, G. (2015). Challenging our most able students. Retrieved from https://www.teachermagazine.com.au/geoff-masters/article/challengingour-most-able-students
McCoach, D., Yu, H., Gottfried, A., & Gottfried, A. (2017). Developing talents: A longitudinal examination of intellectual ability and academic achievement. High Ability Studies, 28 (1), 7–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2017.1298996
Missett, T. C., Brunner, M. M., Callahan, C. M., Moon, T. R., & Price Azano, A. (2014). Exploring teacher beliefs and use of acceleration, ability grouping, and formative assessment. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 37 (3), 245–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162353214541326
Neihart, M. (2007). The socioaffective impact of acceleration and ability grouping: Recommendation for best practice. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51 , 330–341. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986207306319
Neihart, M., & Yeo, L. S. (2018). Psychological issues unique to the gifted student. In S. Pfeiffer (Ed.), APA handbook of giftedness and talent (pp. 497–510). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
New South Wales Department of Education (NSW DET). (2018). Review of selective education access: Findings and action plan. Retrieved from https://education.nsw.gov.au/about-us/strategies-and-reports/our- reports-and-reviews/review-of-selective-education-access/Review-of-Selective-Education-Access.pdf
New South Wales Department of Education (NSW DET). (2019). High potential and gifted education policy . NSW Government. Retrieved from https://education.nsw.gov.au/teaching-and-learning/high-potential-and-gifted- students/high-potential-and-gifted-education-policy
Nicholls, J. G. (1983). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation: A theory and its implications for education. In S. G. Paris, G. M. Olson, & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the classroom (pp. 211–237). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
North, B., & Griffiths, K. (2019). Revisiting gifted education . Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation, NSW Department of Education. Retrieved from https://www.cese.nsw.gov.au/publications-filter/revisiting-gifted-education
North, B., Smith, S. R., & Gross, M. U. M. (2015). Under pressure: The effects of academic pressure on gifted students in high-stakes assessment contexts . Paper presented at the Educating Gifted and Talented Children: Turning Research into Practice, 21st World Council for Gifted and Talented Children Conference, Odense, Denmark.
Plucker, J. A., & Peters, S. J. (2016). Excellence gaps in education: Expanding opportunities for talented students . Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Publishing Group.
Plunkett, M., & Kronborg, L. (2011). Learning to be a teacher of the gifted: The importance of examining opinions and challenging misconceptions. Gifted and Talented International, 26 (1–2), 31–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/15332276.2011.11673587
Poulos, J. (1990). Academic board goes to the movies. Education, 71 (14), 8.
Preckel, F., Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. (2010). Ability grouping of gifted students: Effects on academic self-concept and boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80 , 451–472. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709909X480716
Rasmussen, A., & Lingard, B. (2018). Excellence in education policies: Catering to the needs of gifted and talented or those of self-interest? European Educational Research Journal, 17 (6), 877–897. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474904118771466
Rimlinger, N. (2018). Let’s work together to advance the acceptance and needs of the gifted children in Australia . Retrieved from http://www.aaegt.net.au/wp- content/uploads/Gifted-Awareness-Week-2018.pdf
Rogers, K. B. (1991). The relationship of grouping practices to the education of the gifted . Storrs, CT: National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented.
Rogers, K. B. (2007). Lessons learned about educating the gifted and talented: A synthesis of the research on educational practice. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51 (4), 382–396. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986207306324 . Retrieved from http://aea11gt.pbworks.com/f/LessonsLrnd-Rogers.pdf
Rogers, K. B. (2015). Academic effects research synthesis. In S. Assouline, N. Colangelo, J. VanTassel-Baska, & A. Lupkowski-Shoplik (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students (pp. 19–30). Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Centre for Gifted Education and Talent Development.
Senate Reference Committee (Employment, Workplace Relations, Small Business and Education). (2001). The education of gifted children . Canberra, ACT: Commonwealth of Australia. Retrieved from https://trove.nla.gov.au/work/31974215
Senate Select Committee. (1988). The education of gifted and talented children . Canberra, ACT: Australian Government Publishing Service.
Smith, S. R. (2017). Responding to the unique social and emotional learning needs of gifted Australian students. In E. Frydenberg, A. Martin, & R. Collie (Eds.), Social and emotional learning in Australia and the Asia-Pacific: Perspectives, programs, and approaches (pp. 147–166). Singapore: Springer.
Southern, W. T., & Jones, E. D. (2015). Types of acceleration: Dimensions and issues. In S. G. Assouline, N. Colangelo, J. VanTassel-Baska, & A. Lupkowski-Shoplik (Eds.), A nation empowered (Vol. 2, pp. 9–18). Iowa City, IA: Connie Belin and Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development.
Southern, W. T., Jones, E. D., & Fiscus, E. D. (1989). Practitioner objections to the academic acceleration of gifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 3 (1), 29–35. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698628903300105
Steenbergen-Hu, S., Makel, M. C., & Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2016). What one hundred years of research says about the effects of ability grouping and acceleration on K-12 students’ academic achievement: Findings of two second-order meta-analyses. Review of Educational Research, 86 (4), 849–899. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316675417
Stoeger, H., Hopp, M., & Ziegler, A. (2017). Online mentoring as an extracurricular measure to encourage talented girls in STEM (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics): An empirical study of one-on-one versus group mentoring. Gifted Child Quarterly, 61 , 239–249. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986217702215
Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell, F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: A proposed direction forward based on psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12 (1), 3–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100611418056
VanTassel-Baska, J. (1986). Acceleration. In C. J. Maker (Ed.), Critical issues in gifted education (pp. 179–196). Rockville, MD: Aspen Publications.
VanTassel-Baska, J. (1992). Educational decision making on acceleration and grouping. Gifted Child Quarterly, 36 (2), 68–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/001698629203600203
VanTassel-Baska, J., & Brown, E. F. (2009). An analysis of gifted education curriculum models. In F. A. Karnes & S. M. Bean (Eds.), Methods and materials for teaching the gifted (pp. 75–106). Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
Vinson, T. (2002). Inquiry into the provision of public education in NSW: Report of the “Vinson inquiry” . Annandale, NSW/Darlinghurst, NSW: New South Wales Teachers’ Federation/Pluto Press. Retrieved from https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/19872236
Vogl, K., & Preckel, F. (2014). Full-time ability grouping of gifted students: Impacts on social self-concept and school-related attitudes. Gifted Child Quarterly, 58 (1), 51–68. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986213513795
White, S., Graham, L., & Blaas, S. (2018). Why do we know so little about the factors associated with gifted underachievement? A systematic literature review. Educational Research Review, 24 , 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2018.03.001
Young, M., Rogers, K., Hoekman, K., van Vliet, H., & Chan, L. C. (2015). Acceleration in Australia: Flexible pacing opens the way for early university admission. In S. Assouline, N. Colangelo, J. VanTassel-Baska, & A. Lupkowski-Shoplik (Eds.), A nation empowered: Evidence trumps the excuses holding back America’s brightest students (pp. 225–240). Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Centre for Gifted Education and Talent Development. Retrieved from https://files.nwesd.org/website/Teaching_Learning/HiCap/2015-16%20meetings/NationEmpowered%20Vol1.pdf
Download references
Authors and affiliations.
GERRIC, School of Education, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Miraca U. M. Gross & Susen R. Smith
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Correspondence to Susen R. Smith .
Editors and affiliations.
School of Education, UNSW Australia, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Susen R. Smith
National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore
Rhoda Myra Garces-Bacsal
Reprints and permissions
© 2019 Crown
Cite this entry.
Gross, M.U.M., Smith, S.R. (2019). Put Them Together and See How They Learn! Ability Grouping and Acceleration Effects on the Self-Esteem of Academically Gifted High School Students. In: Smith, S. (eds) Handbook of Giftedness and Talent Development in the Asia-Pacific. Springer International Handbooks of Education. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-3021-6_17-1
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-3021-6_17-1
Received : 21 July 2019
Accepted : 15 August 2019
Published : 01 October 2019
Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore
Print ISBN : 978-981-13-3021-6
Online ISBN : 978-981-13-3021-6
eBook Packages : Springer Reference Education Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Education
Policies and ethics
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Ability-grouping in primary schools: case studies and critical debates. Patrick Barmby. Pages 91-94 | Published online: 01 Nov 2017. Cite this article. https://doi.org/10.1080/14794802.2017.1365007. Full Article. Figures & data. References. Citations. Metrics. Reprints & Permissions.
Students’ ability group placements in the early grades evolve into divergent educational paths that grow further apart with multiple years of grouping. These findings provide the first longitudinal evidence linking ability grouping to the reproduction of educational inequalities.
It is common practice within primary classrooms for teachers to spilt children into different ability groups so that children of similar level are taught together. Whilst this practice is used across the globe, research is mixed on the benefits of such grouping strategy.
Two second-order meta-analyses synthesized approximately 100 years of research on the effects of ability grouping and acceleration on K–12 students’ academic achievement.
The findings will draw on three intensive case-studies which explore children's experiences of ability grouping focusing on learner identity (see McGillicuddy & Devine, 2020), psychosocial responses, affinity with school and patterns of social interaction.
Using case study evidence, this article demonstrates that children’s lived experiences of ‘ability’ are highly individual and shaped by a broader range of social, structural and pedagogic ...
examine in detail how ability grouping operated in three primary schools, providing evidence from observations and interviews with staff and pupils to identify some of the implications of these practices.
Linda Grant , and James Rothenberg The Social Enhancement of Ability Differences: Teacher-Student Interactions in First-And Second-Grade Reading Groups, The Elementary School Journal 87, no.1 1 (Oct 2015): 29–49.
Specifically, a study will be discussed that examined the effects of three interventions: (1) ability grouping, (2) acceleration, and (3) acceleration used concurrently with ability grouping, on the self-esteem of academically gifted high school students.
In a study of within-classroom grouping in first-grade classrooms, Weinstein (1976) found that assignment to a high group positively affected children's academic self-concepts, while assignment to a low group did not. Borg (1966) also found that low-ability pupils had lower self-concepts in ability-grouped schools than in non-grouped schools.